How Parents Should Teach Media Literacy
http://digitalcommons.uri.edu/jmle/vol8/iss2/1/
Many parents fail to interact with their children regularly about media content.
What is the relationship between parents’ critical thinking about media and parents’ provision of both active and restrictive mediation of television content.
Results revealed that parents’ critical thinking about media is positively associated with both active and restrictive mediation, relationships mediated by parents’ attitudes toward parent-child interactions about media.
Media literacy programs aimed at improving parents’ critical thinking about media may be an effective way to alter children’s responses to media exposure and that these media literacy programs should promote positive attitudes toward parental mediation.
Research shows that parent-child media-related interactions—often referred to as parental mediation behaviors—are an effective way to alter the influence of media exposure on children’s well-being (Austin, Hust, & Kistler, 2009; Chakroff & Nathanson, 2008; Nathanson, 2001). Due to consistent empirical evidence showing parental mediation enhances positive effects and prevents negative effects of media exposure on children’s well-being (Buijzen & Valkenburg, 2005; Chakroff & Nathanson, 2008), scholars, educators, and pediatricians have called on parents to involve themselves more actively in discussing and monitoring their children’s media use (AAP, 2011; Brown, Shifrin, & Hill, 2015). Despite recommendations, the majority of youth in the United States report they do not function under any rules or restrictions from their parents regarding the type or amount of television content they can watch (Rideout, Foehr, & Roberts, 2010). Furthermore, parents report that they talk with their children about television even less than they set rules (Vittrup, 2009). In order to influence children’s responses to media exposure positively, researchers should determine what can be done to identify factors that predict parents’ motivations to be more involved with their children’s media use.
Previous research found parents who engage in parental mediation tend to be motivated by negative attitudes about media content and by concern about potentially negative effects of media exposure (Bybee, Robinson, & Turow, 1982; Mendoza, 2009).
Some of the goals of adult media literacy programs (see Leivens, 2015) include increasing one’s ability to think critically about media (Austin, Pinkleton, Radanielina-Hita, & Ran, 2015) and empowering parents to make good choices about their children’s media exposure through increased knowledge about the potential effects of media exposure (AAP, 2011). It is possible that parents with greater ability to think critically about media are more likely to engage in parental mediation. The present study investigated the relationship between parents’ critical thinking about media, attitudes about parent-child interactions about media, and parental mediation behaviors.
Media Literacy among Parents
The present study conceptualizes media literacy as one’s ability to decode, evaluate, and analyze media content in a variety of formats (Aufderheide, 1993; Center for Media Literacy, 2015). Most media literacy intervention research involves programs aimed at improving the media literacy skills of children, including the ability to analyze media content and sources critically (Austin et al., 2015). In order to teach media literacy skills to children, adults (i.e., parents and teachers) should have higher levels of media literacy, although this is not always the case (Potter, 2014). Teachers, those most often involved with implementing media literacy programs in schools, receive little— if any—media education and rarely discuss media literacy concepts in training or staff development (Kellner & Share, 2005). Similarly, pediatricians receive little media literacy training, as less than one-third of accredited pediatrician residency programs teach their students about the influences of media exposure on the well-being of children and adolescents (Rich & Bar-on, 2001). Pediatricians lacking training on media literacy seems understandable given the wealth of other information needed to be conveyed during residency, but that leaves only one other group of adults with the potential to regularly interact with children about media, and this is parents.
Parental media literacy levels may be even lower than those of teachers and pediatricians because parents rely on the expertise of child development and healthcare professionals to provide information for parenting decisions (Rich, 2014). In addition, parental mediation efforts are often not based on theory or research, but rather parents’ personal experiences (Potter, 2010). Parents are relatively unaware of the effects that media messages can have on attitudes and behaviors, whereas such awareness is present among people with higher levels of media literacy (Potter, 2010). Parents also tend to believe their own children are at less risk of being affected by exposure to sexual and violent media content than other children (Meirick, Sims, Gilchrist, & Croucher, 2009; Nathanson, Eveland, Park, & Paul, 2002; Tsfati, Ribak, & Cohen, 2005). A lack of awareness of potential media effects corresponds with parents’ lack of concern about media content to which their children are exposed. For example, survey research found that most adults are not bothered “a lot” by violent and sexual media content in the U.S. (Pew, 2005) and the U.K. (Ofcom, 2014). Interviews with parents have found a similar lack of concern (Jordan, Hersey, McDivitt, & Heitzler, 2006).
If parents have low levels of media literacy, then one may expect their children to have little access to developing their own media literacy skills, as parents are in the best position to influence their children’s responses to media exposure (Browne, 1999; Hogan, 2001). Indeed, scholars have called for the provision of information about media content to parents in order to influence children’s media exposure more positively (Bushman & Cantor, 2003). When parents do make attempts to influence children’s responses to media exposure, parental mediation takes place.
Parental Mediation
Parental mediation refers to parent-child interactions about media and generally encompasses three types of activities: active mediation, restrictive mediation, and coviewing. Active mediation refers to parent-child conversations about media and media content. Restrictive mediation refers to parent-enacted rules and restrictions related to children’s media use. Coviewing refers to parents consuming media messages with their children (Nathanson, 2001; Valkenburg, Krcmar, Peeters, & Marseille, 1999). Parents who engage in consistent parental mediation do so with the intent to impact how, and the extent to which, media exposure influences children (Rasmussen, 2013). Parents concerned with media content and its effects employ parental mediation behaviors to protect their children’s well-being (Nathanson et al., 2002). Researchers regard parental mediation as the enactment of parental media literacy (Mendoza, 2009) or as the sharing of parental media literacy skills with children (Austin et al., 2015). There are, however, few studies examining the relationship between parental media literacy and parental mediation activities. As Mendoza (2009) notes, “media literacy and the field of parent media education lack conversation with the parental mediation literature” (p. 29).
Only a handful of studies explore aspects of the relationship between parental media literacy and parental mediation practices. For example, a survey of parents found that media literacy, represented largely by the parents’ critical thinking toward media sources and content, was positively associated with parents’ expectations that any future conversations they might have with their children about certain media messages would positively influence children’s well-being (Austin et al., 2015). In addition, the ability of parents to assess food advertising critically associated positively with more parent-child discussions about advertising claims (Hindin, Contento, & Gussow, 2004). The extant literature currently lacks research that examines the relationship between parents’ ability to think critically about media and parental mediation of children’s television exposure. Past research does suggest, however, that the relationship between parents’ critical thinking about media, as one indicator of parental media literacy, and parental mediation is likely mediated by parents’ attitudes toward these media-related interactions.
Attitudes Toward Media-Related Interactions with Children
Because of the strength of parents’ influence on children (Pequegnat & Szapocznik, 2000), many interventions designed to alter children’s behaviors first attempt to change parenting behaviors, which, in turn, have been shown to alter children’s attitudes and behaviors (Hutchinson & Wood, 2007). These communication interventions aim to bolster protective parenting behaviors and skills by altering parents’ knowledge about the risks their children face and by altering parents’ attitudes about parent-child communication and other protective parenting behaviors (see, e.g., Maria, Markham, Bluethmann, & Mullen, 2015). A review of these interventions found that programs often succeed at improving parents’ attitudes toward sexual health communication and, ultimately, at increasing parent-child sexual health communication (Maria et al., 2015). Such increased engagement with these protective communication behaviors appears to result from the interventions’ influence on parents’ attitudes toward protective behaviors. Similarly, we should expect parents who are more able to think critically about media to have more positive attitudes toward those behaviors that they know will decrease their children’s susceptibility to negative effects of media exposure, such as parental mediation, than those who are less able to think critically about media. In this study, both active and restrictive mediation are considered protective parental mediation behaviors. Similar to past research, co-viewing was excluded from this study because it is relatively ineffective at protecting children from undesirable media effects (see Valkenburg, Piotrowski, Hermanns, & de Leeuw, 2013) and because co-viewing is more common among parents who have a positive attitude about television (see, e.g., Austin & Pinkleton, 2001; Dorr, Kovaric, & Doubleday, 1989; Nathanson, 2001). Therefore, we predicted:
H1: Parents’ critical thinking about media will be positively associated with their attitudes toward (a) active and (b) restrictive mediation.
Attitude-Behavior Relationship
Attitudes toward behaviors consistently predict behavioral intentions (for a review see Ajzen, 2012) and behaviors themselves (see, e.g., Kraus, 1995; McEachan, Conner, Taylor, & Lawton, 2011). If attitudes about behavior can be changed, then behaviors are likely also modifiable (Smith & Mackie, 2007). For example, theories predicting behavior through a reasoned action approach (see Ajzen, 2012) suggest that attitudes are highly predictive of behaviors.
Attitudes are often stronger predictors of behaviors than other constructs such as subjective norms (O’Keefe, 2002).
The consistent relationship between attitudes and behaviors has been demonstrated in a broad range of parenting research, beginning as early as when adults make a decision to become a parent. For example, adults’ positive attitudes about childbearing correlate to rates of marital childbearing (Barber, 2001). Parents’ attitudes toward using medicine to reduce a child’s fever are also related to their intentions to use medication when a child has a fever (Walsh, Edwards, & Fraser, 2009). Past research indicates that parents are more likely to intend to engage in parent-child communication if they have positive attitudes toward parent-child communication (Hutchinson & Wood, 2007). And, although parents’ attitudes about media content are highly predictive of parental mediation (Valkenburg et al., 1999; Warren, 2001), no research to date investigates the relationship between attitudes toward parental mediation and actual provision of parental mediation. Therefore, the present study tests the following predictions:
- H2: Parents’ positive attitudes toward active mediation will be positively associated with the frequency of their provision of active mediation.
- H3: Parents’ attitudes toward restrictive mediation will be positively associated with the frequency of their provision of restrictive mediation.
By combining the previous hypotheses, we are able to construct a proposed mediation model predicting that parents’ critical thinking about media will be positively associated with the frequency of their provision of parental mediation through its relationship with parents’ attitudes toward parental mediation. Specifically, we predicted:
- H4: Parents’ critical thinking about media will be positively associated with parents’ positive attitudes toward active mediation, which will, in turn, be associated with the frequency of parents’ provision of active mediation.
- H5: Parents’ critical thinking about media will be positively associated with parents’ positive attitudes toward restrictive mediation, which will, in turn, be associated with the frequency of parents’ provision of restrictive mediation.Method
Results
Hypothesis 1 predicted that parents’ critical thinking about media would be positively associated with their attitudes about active and restrictive mediation. Hierarchical linear regression with child age, child gender, parentage, parent gender, parent education, and income entered in the first block; parents’ attitudes about violent and sexual television/movie content entered in the second block; parents’ and children’s television/movie exposure entered in the third block; and parents’ critical thinking about media entered in the fourth block indicated that parents’ critical thinking about media accounted for 4.6% of the variance in attitudes toward active mediation (See Table 2). Specifically, parents with higher levels of critical thinking about media were more likely to have positive attitudes about active mediation (β = .213, p < .001). The same analysis with attitudes toward restrictive mediation as the dependent variable revealed that parents’ critical thinking about media accounted for 2.4% of the variance in attitudes toward restrictive mediation (see Table 2). Specifically, parents with higher levels of critical thinking about media were more likely to have positive attitudes about restrictive mediation (β = .158, p = .02). Therefore, results support H1.
Hypothesis 2 predicted that parents’ positive attitudes toward active mediation would be positively associated with their provision of active mediation. Hierarchical linear regression with child age, child gender, parent age, parent gender, parent education, and income entered in the first block; parents’ attitudes about violent and sexual television/movie content entered in the second block; parents’ and children’s television/movie exposure entered in the third block; and parents’ attitudes toward active mediation entered in the fourth block revealed that attitudes toward active mediation accounted for 5.9% of the variance in active mediation (see Table 3). Specifically, parents with more positive attitudes toward active mediation reported more frequent provision of active mediation (β = .167, p = .001). Therefore, results also supported H2.
Hypothesis 3 predicted that parents’ positive attitudes toward restrictive mediation would be positively associated with their provision of restrictive mediation. Hierarchical linear regression with the same variables as the previous analysis entered in the first three blocks, and with parents’ attitudes toward restrictive mediation in the fourth block revealed that attitudes toward restrictive mediation accounted for 15.5% of the variance in restrictive mediation (see Table 3). Specifically, parents with more positive attitudes toward restrictive mediation reported more frequent provision of restrictive mediation (β = .280, p < .001). Therefore, regression analyses supported H3.
Hypothesis 4 predicted that parents’ positive attitudes toward active mediation would mediate the relationship between parents’ critical thinking about media and active mediation. The simple mediation model (see Figure 1) between critical thinking about media and active mediation through attitudes toward active mediation was estimated using OLS regression, with covariates of: child age, child gender, parentage, parent gender, parent education, income, parent and child television/movie exposure, and attitudes toward violent and sexual TV content. Specifically, we employed Hayes’ (2013) PROCESS SPSS macro (model 4), which estimates both direct and indirect effects of simple mediation models. The total effect (c) of parents’ critical thinking about media on active mediation was significant (b = .099, t = 2.45, p = .01, 95% CI [.0194, .1788]). The direct effect (c’), removing the effect of the mediator, was not significant (b = .068, t = 1.67, p = .10, 95% CI [-.0127, .1490]). The indirect effect through attitudes toward active mediation, however, was significant (b = .031, 95% CI [.0078, .0681]). Therefore, the mediation analysis supported H4.
Hypothesis 5 predicted that parents’ positive attitudes toward restrictive mediation would mediate the relationship between parents’ critical thinking about media and restrictive mediation. The simple mediation model (see Figure 2) between critical thinking about media and restrictive mediation through attitudes toward restrictive mediation was estimated using OLS regression, with covariates of: child age, child gender, parentage, parent gender, parent education, income, parent and child television/movie exposure, and attitudes toward violent and sexual TV content. Again using Hayes’ (2013) PROCESS SPSS macro (model 4), results revealed that the total effect (c) of parents’ critical thinking about media on restrictive mediation was not significant (b = .077, t = 1.89, p = .06, 95% CI [- .0033, .1581]). The direct effect (c’), removing the effect of the mediator, also was not significant (b = .035, t = .92, p = .36, 95% CI [-.0397, .1088]). The indirect effect through attitudes toward restrictive mediation, however, was significant (b = .043, 95% CI [.0084, .0818]). Therefore, the mediation analysis supported H5.
Discussion
Research consistently shows that parental mediation is effective at altering children’s responses to media exposure (Chakroff & Nathanson, 2008) and experts encourage parents to increase the amount of parental mediation they provide (AAP, 2011). Empirical research suggests, however, that many children do not receive frequent parental mediation (Rideout et al., 2010; Vittrup, 2009). Results from the current study indicate parents with greater ability to think critically about media may be more likely to engage in parental mediation behaviors. Additionally, the present study found that parents’ attitudes toward active and restrictive mediation mediate the relationship between parents’ critical thinking about media and their mediation behaviors. Despite the effectiveness of parental mediation, some suggest that relying on parents to guide children’s media experiences is not an effective strategy due to the paucity of parents’ technical expertise, time, and communication skills (Livingstone, Van Couvering, & Thumim, 2005).
This study provides initial evidence that parental media literacy, conceptualized in the present study as critical thinking about media sources and content, is a factor that predicts whether or not parents engage in active and restrictive mediation of television content. These results suggest that the encouragement provided by educators and policymakers for parents to provide parental mediation may not be sufficient to empower parents in their efforts to guide their children’s media use. Instead, the necessary empowerment may come from participation in adult media literacy programs (Leivens, 2015) that train parents how to evaluate media sources and content critically, in addition to teaching them about the role of parents in altering children’s responses to media exposure (Livingstone et al., 2005). Such media literacy programs, by increasing adults’ ability to process media messages logically and critically, may also give adults at least some of the tools necessary to effectively communicate with their children about media and media content. Results of this study suggest that these programs should also include lessons aimed at improving parents’ attitudes about parental mediation in order to ultimately motivate parents to engage in parental mediation behaviors.
Results show that the relationship between parents’ critical thinking about media and the frequency of their provision of both active and restrictive mediation is mediated by parents’ attitudes toward parental mediation. Parents who are better able to think critically about media reported more positive attitudes about participating in media-related parent-child interactions, and these positive attitudes, in turn, are associated with more frequent provision of parental mediation. Thinking critically about media content and sources can help increase one’s skepticism toward media messages (Austin, Pinkleton, Hust, & Cohen, 2005) and such critical thinking and skepticism can result in a desire to protect oneself and one’s children from messages that could negatively influence their well-being (Nathanson et al., 2002). In other words, parents with greater ability to think critically about media will likely engage in more frequent parent-child interactions about media use. While the present study is consistent with a substantial body of research establishing such a relationship between attitudes toward behaviors and subsequent behaviors, the current study is unique in that it provides empirical evidence of the relationship between attitudes toward parental mediation and actual parental mediation behaviors. Future research should explore these associations within a framework, such as protection motivation theory, capable of elucidating the cognitive pathways that may mediate the relations found herein.
In order to understand whether or not parents engage in regular parental mediation, some extant parental mediation research has focused on identifying the precursors of mediation efforts. Although findings from the current study are correlational in nature, they suggest that parents’ critical thinking about media may be an important precursor to the provision of parental mediation. While the study’s findings offer opportunities for future media literacy efforts, some limitations merit discussion.
First, the data are correlational in nature, and as such, cannot establish causality. Perhaps parents’ engagement in parental mediation activities encourages them to become more critical media consumers. It is possible that these behaviors lead to more positive attitudes about parental mediation in a manner consistent with the way people change their attitudes to make them consistent with their behaviors, as posited by dissonance theory (see, e.g., Harmon-Jones & Mills, 1999). Although the causal ordering of the mediation models in this study cannot be definitively established without longitudinal data, the cited literature alludes to the plausibility of the proposed relations. Future longitudinal or experimental research should investigate these possibilities. In addition, parents’ critical thinking about media is only one indicator of the larger construct of media literacy. Future research should explore other aspects of parental media literacy and their relationship with parental mediation. Last, study participants were those who had access to the Internet and were familiar with taking online surveys. It is possible that participants’ responses differ from those that might be provided by people less familiar with the Internet or who have limited Internet access. Future research should employ data collection methods that can capture both online and offline populations.
The present study provides an initial answer to the call for the creation of conversations between the fields of media literacy and parental mediation. As far as we know, this is the first study to show a positive association between parents’ critical thinking about media and the frequency of their provision of parental mediation. Further, parents’ critical thinking about media appears to influence parental mediation behaviors by first influencing attitudes about parental mediation, a pattern that is consistent with decades of research on attitude-behavior relations. Policymakers, educators, and pediatricians should use this knowledge to influence children’s responses to media exposure by developing adult media literacy programs that are capable of empowering parents to engage in parental mediation activities.